Purports of brain damage following presumed whiplash injury

Issue: BCMJ, vol. 44 , No. 6 , July August 2002 , Pages 303-306 Clinical Articles

There are cases of purported whiplash whereby the victim claims to have suffered brain damage, based in part on apparent changes to his or her mental status. Efforts to confirm the presence of brain damage with objective testing have failed to establish a physical cause for the claimed mental disturbances. Those mental disorders found post-whiplash have not shown the patterns that have been associated with traumatic brain disorders, but rather with impairment of attentional functioning, presumably due to psychosocial factors.


Brain lesions are expected only in whiplash injuries involving a high magnitude of acceleration-deceleration, and are unlikely to occur without significant loss of consciousness and peritraumatic amnesia.


Mild traumatic head injury, with or without direct impact to the head, is a possible consequence of an acceleration-deceleration mechanism of injury (whiplash).[1] Patients may present without a history of significant loss of consciousness and may not demonstrate any short-duration superficial loss of consciousness. Although there may be numerous short-lasting (days) somatic, psychological, or cognitive symptoms following mild whiplash trauma,[1-4] a tissue damage cause, and a basis on which these symptoms may be demonstrated over a prolonged period of time (months), is a matter of controversy. Probably the most prominent debate concerns the very notion that whiplash trauma can lead to any physical brain damage.[5,6]

Patients who purport to have suffered mild traumatic brain injury as a result of a whiplash-type mechanism usually show normal findings on physical, neurological, and/or MRI examination.[1,4,7] But these are not conclusive tests. For this reason functional examinations such as neuropsychological testing and/or neuroimaging studies such as positron emission tomography (PET) or single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT) may be expected to provide conclusive evidence of brain damage. Such tests, if positive in demonstrating disturbed function of brain structures, should also correlate to psychological or cognitive symptoms before a presumption of traumatic brain damage can be made.

Localization of trauma-induced cognitive impairment

In a proportion of individuals who had suffered whiplash trauma, the principal cognitive impairment was identified within the complex of attentional processing (i.e., concentration, attention, or working memory).[8-9] Could this be due to physical brain damage? Several experimental studies have shown that for unimpaired complex attentional processing, an intact prefrontal cortex is necessary.[10-12] Based on this, if brain damage is the basis of impaired cognitive functioning following mild whiplash trauma, there should be positive neuroimaging findings in the rostral brain areas (frontal or temporal pole, prefrontal cortex).

Brain damage and whiplash

Animal experiments have shown that the likelihood of brain damage following whiplash-type trauma, without direct head impact, will mainly depend on the magnitude of acceleration-deceleration forces and the rotation forces acting upon the skull.[13,14] However, there was not a linear relationship between acceleration-deceleration forces and the size of brain damage.[15] In addition, it was proposed that rotation of the skull is an essential element in the forces that might provoke loss of consciousness.[13] Accordingly, in the clinical assessment of unconsciousness, the most important indicator of possible brain damage is the severity of the trauma. Minor trauma is an unlikely candidate.

Both in experimental research with animals and in postmortem studies with patients who had previously suffered whiplash-like trauma, but died because of a different reason, morphological lesions were found primarily in the periphery of rostral brain areas. The distribution of rostral lesions was observed almost proportionately from the periphery to more central parts of the brain, with the greater traumatic effects being inflicted upon the periphery. Consequently lesions of more central parts of the brain may be expected only in quite severe trauma in which lesions to the periphery will also occur.[14] Based on the tangential acceleration effects caused by a rotation of the skull, a peripheral shearing effect upon brain tissue may occur, which may lead to a diffuse axonal injury that is more profuse at the periphery.[16,17] This may be explained as follows: due to shearing, neurons and small vessels may rupture. Following rupture of neurons, intracellular substances such as the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate will be released into the intercellular space. Glutamate will lead to an excitation of the additional neurons near the site of primary lesion, which may be excited to death (so-called excitotoxicity).[18] This may lead to a kind of chain reaction that induces death of neurons not primarily damaged by the trauma; for this reason diffuse axonal injury is not limited only to the immediate site of initial damage, but may also include other nearby tissue.

It may be assumed that the rupture of neurons may result in a time-limited interruption of the neuronal network, which may explain loss of consciousness in patients following head trauma. It may be considered that the duration and depth of unconsciousness, including the phenomenon called post-traumatic amnesia, may depend on the extent of the damage (the size of disrupted neuronal network). Consequently, after any type of head or whiplash-type trauma, patients who did not lose consciousness for even a short duration, or suffer any short post-traumatic amnesia, probably did not suffer a diffuse axonal injury.[19] Accordingly in these cases, brain damage could not be observed using neuroimaging (PET or SPECT), nor could be assumed on the basis of cognitive deficits.

Neuropsychological and neuroimaging following whiplash

In the region where diffuse axonal injury occurred, changes in tissue metabolism may be expected, and glucose, the most important energy source of the brain, will no longer be utilized.[20] In contrast, brain areas that are still active—such as the speech area during vocalization—will require more energy. In addition, the energy requirements in the brain tissue are highly correlated to tissue perfusion.[21] Accordingly, in the brain tissue where damage has occurred, both diminished glucose metabolism and perfusion should be observed. In a living organism, brain glucose metabolism may be assessed using 18-fluorodeoxyglucose with PET, whereas tissue perfusion may be evaluated using 99Tc-Hexamethylpropylenaminoxim (HMPAO) with SPECT. As a cautionary note, both methods show considerable interindividual variations based on functional status (such as whether the person is vocalizing, the person’s age,[22] and whether the person is anxious or depressed).[23] Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that cognitive problems during the course of a depression may be observed by functional neuroimaging (PET).[24] The latter should be considered while assessing patients following whiplash trauma who may demonstrate changes in emotional functioning.[4]

In some previous studies in patients who suffered a whiplash trauma, significant reductions in both metabolism and perfusion were found bilaterally in the parieto-occipital regions.[5,6] This was initially presumed to be evidence of brain damage. However, the above localization of test anomalies was found in regions of the brain that previously were not considered important for complex functional attention. Although impaired complex functional attention has been observed following whiplash trauma,[8,9] lesions in the parieto-occipital region were not identified.[13,16,17] In one study of post-whiplash patients, reductions of metabolism and perfusion in parieto-occipital regions were identified to reflect the thickness of cortex in these areas of the brain, and therefore reflected an anatomical variation of the morphologically unimpaired brain.[25] In this research, deficits of metabolism and perfusion were also demonstrated bilaterally in frontal pole and putamen. More important, in these same patients there were no significant correlations between scoring of functional attention and neuroimaging findings.[26] An additional study with patients after bona fide mild and moderate traumatic brain injury (loss of consciousness including post-traumatic amnesia less than 5 minutes) demonstrated perfusion deficits in frontal brain areas,[27] the same area as observed in animals and discussed above. Interestingly, in these patients, the observed frontal hypoperfusion was shown to improve at follow-up investigation,[27] which correlates to follow-up studies regarding attentional functioning in which improvement has also been observed over time.[28] These findings overall support the view that if or when impaired attentional functioning follows mild head or whiplash-type trauma, it may be based on a time-limited but fully reversible functional brain impairment; minor brain damage does not cause lasting dysfunction.

Summary

Experimental and clinical research has shown that the severity of the trauma is clearly correlated to the degree of the brain damage. A morphological brain lesion following whiplash-type trauma may be expected if the acceleration-deceleration forces achieved high magnitude. If this magnitude is achieved, lesions will primarily occur in the rostral brain areas (prefrontal cortex, frontal or temporal pole). Very high acceleration-deceleration forces may also lead to more diffuse lesions even in the deeper brain structures. The size of the lesion is in direct correlation to the duration of the loss of consciousness, the depth of coma, and the duration of post-traumatic amnesia. The basis of the impaired consciousness is likely due to diffuse axonal injury. Without significant loss of consciousness and peritraumatic amnesia, brain damage does not occur. Brain regions that are exposed to damage following acceleration-deceleration trauma are the prefrontal cortex, which is crucial for the more complex attentional functioning. Impairment of complex attentional processing may be observed to parallel the more severe trauma cases, so indicators of trauma severity (duration loss and depth of consciousness, post-traumatic amnesia) should be in a significant relationship to the clinical cognitive impairment and neuroimaging findings (PET). If these relationships cannot be established, factors other than brain damage (pain, adverse effect of medication, psychological or psychosocial problems) should be considered as the basis of complaints following purported whiplash trauma. Patients making claims of brain damage but without the prerequisite unconsciousness and peritraumatic amnesia should not be considered bogus; they are still patients, but their therapeutic approach is probably best focused on the psychological and behavioral aspects of their life.

Competing interests

None declared.


References

1. Alexander MP. Mild traumatic brain injury: Pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical management. Neurology 1995;45:1253-1260. PubMed Citation
2. Levin HS, Mattis S, Ruff M, et al. Neurobehavioral outcome following minor head injury: a three-center study. J Neurosurg 1987;66:234-243. PubMed Abstract 
3. Evans RW. The postconcussional syndrome and the sequelae of mild head injury. Neurol Clin 1992;10:815-847. PubMed Abstract
4. Radanov BP, Sturzenegger M, Di Stefano G. Long-term outcome after whiplash injury—A two year follow-up considering features of accident mechanism, somatic, radiological and psychosocial findings. Medicine 1995;74:281-297. PubMed Abstract
5. Otte A, Mueller-Brand J, Fierz L. Brain SPECT findings in late whiplash syndrome. Lancet 1995;345:1513-1514. PubMed Citation
6. Otte A, Ettlin T, Fierz L, et al. Parieto-occipital hypoperfusion in late whiplash syndrome: First quantitative SPET study using technetium-99m bicisate (ECD). Eur J Nucl Med 1996;23:72-74. PubMed Abstract
7. Sturzenegger M, DiStefano G, Radanov BP, et al. Presenting symptoms and signs after whiplash injury: The influence of accident mechanisms. Neurology 1994;44:688-693. PubMed Abstract
8. Mattson AJ, Levin HS. Frontal lobe dysfunction following closed head injury. A review of the literature. J Nerv Ment Dis 1990;178:282-291. PubMed Abstract
9. Stuss DT, Gow CA. Frontal dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychiatr Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1992;5:272-282. 
10. Goldman-Rakic PS. The issue of memory in the study of prefrontal function. In: Thierry AM, Glowinski J, Goldman-Rakic PS, et al. (eds). Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex. New York, NY: Springer, 1994, S:112-121. 
11. D’Esposito MD, Detre JA, Alsop DC, et al. The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 1995;378:279-281. PubMed Abstract
12. Fiez JA, Raife EA, Balota DA, et al. A positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of verbal information. J Neurosci 1996;16:808-822. PubMed Abstract
13. Ommaya K, Gennarelli T. Experimental head injury. In: Vinken P, Bruyn G (eds). Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company, 1975:67-90. 
14. Gennarelli TA, Thibault LE, Adams H, et al. Diffuse axonal injury and traumatic coma in primate. Ann Neurol 1982;12:564-574. PubMed Abstract
15. Elson LM, Ward CC. Mechanisms and pathophysiology of mild head injury. Semi Neurol 1994;14:8-18. PubMed Citation
16. Gennarelli TA. Mechanisms of brain injury. J Emerg Med 1993;11 Suppl 1:5-11. PubMed Abstract
17. Oppenheimer DR. Microscopic lesions in the brain following head injury. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1968;31:299-306. PubMed Citation
18. Hayes RL, Dixon CE. Neurochemical changes in mild head injury. Semin Neurol 1994;14:25-31. PubMed Citation
19. Esselman PC, Uomoto JM. Classification of the spectrum of mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 1995; 9:417-24. PubMed Abstract
20. Pentreath VW, Seal LH, Morrison JH, et al. Transmitter mediated regulation of energy metabolism in nervous tissue at the cellular level. Neurochem Int 1986;9:1-10. 
21. Bryan RM. Cerebral blood flow and energy metabolism during stress. Am J Physiol 1990;259:H269-280. PubMed Abstract
22. Loessner A, Alavi A, Lewandrowski K-U, et al. Regional cerebral function determined by FDG-PET in healthy volunteers: Normal patterns and change with age. J Nucl Med 1995;36:1141-1149. PubMed Abstract
23. Dolan RJ, Bench CJ, Brown RG, et al. Neuropsychological dysfunction in depression: The relationship to regional cerebral blood flow. Psychol Med 1994;24:849-857. PubMed Abstract
24. Dolan RJ, Bench CJ, Brown RG. Regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in depressed patients with cognitive impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992;55:768-773. PubMed Abstract
25. Bicik I, Radanov BP, Schäfer N, et al. PET with 18fluorodeoxyglucose and hexamethylpropylene amine oxime SPECT in late whiplash syndrome. Neurology 1998;51:345-350. PubMed Abstract
26. Radanov BP, Bicik I, Dvorak J, et al. Relation between neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings in patients with late whiplash syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;66:485-489. PubMed Abstract
27. Jacobs A, Put E, Ingles M, et al. Prospective evaluation of technetium-99m-HMPAO SPECT in mild and moderate traumatic brain injury. J Nucl Med 1994;35:942-947. PubMed Abstract
28. Gronwall D, Wrightson P. Delayed recovery of intellectual function after minor head injury. Lancet 1974;2:605-609. PubMed Citation

 


Bogdan P. Radanov, MD

Dr Radanov is an associate professor of psychiatry at the University of Berne, Switzerland.

Bogdan P. Radanov, MD. Purports of brain damage following presumed whiplash injury. BCMJ, Vol. 44, No. 6, July, August, 2002, Page(s) 303-306 - Clinical Articles.



Above is the information needed to cite this article in your paper or presentation. The International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) recommends the following citation style, which is the now nearly universally accepted citation style for scientific papers:
Halpern SD, Ubel PA, Caplan AL, Marion DW, Palmer AM, Schiding JK, et al. Solid-organ transplantation in HIV-infected patients. N Engl J Med. 2002;347:284-7.

About the ICMJE and citation styles

The ICMJE is small group of editors of general medical journals who first met informally in Vancouver, British Columbia, in 1978 to establish guidelines for the format of manuscripts submitted to their journals. The group became known as the Vancouver Group. Its requirements for manuscripts, including formats for bibliographic references developed by the U.S. National Library of Medicine (NLM), were first published in 1979. The Vancouver Group expanded and evolved into the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE), which meets annually. The ICMJE created the Recommendations for the Conduct, Reporting, Editing, and Publication of Scholarly Work in Medical Journals to help authors and editors create and distribute accurate, clear, easily accessible reports of biomedical studies.

An alternate version of ICMJE style is to additionally list the month an issue number, but since most journals use continuous pagination, the shorter form provides sufficient information to locate the reference. The NLM now lists all authors.

BCMJ standard citation style is a slight modification of the ICMJE/NLM style, as follows:

  • Only the first three authors are listed, followed by "et al."
  • There is no period after the journal name.
  • Page numbers are not abbreviated.


For more information on the ICMJE Recommendations for the Conduct, Reporting, Editing, and Publication of Scholarly Work in Medical Journals, visit www.icmje.org

BCMJ Guidelines for Authors

Leave a Reply